Autism Spectrum Disorder

Patricia Howlin (UK)

Abstract

In recent years there have been many improvements in our knowledge and understanding of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The following article provides an update on prevalence data, theories of causation, and recent changes to diagnostic classification systems. Assessment instruments that can be used to identify individuals at risk of ASD and/or to assist in the diagnostic process are discussed. Evidence for the effectiveness of many intervention programmes remains limited although interventions with a focus on early parent-child relationships have a growing evidence base. The generally poor outcome for adults with ASD, including those of average or above intellectual ability is highlighted and possible ways of improving outcome in the future are considered.

Key words

Autism; Autism spectrum disorders; diagnosis; causes; intervention; adult outcomes.

Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a pervasive developmental disorder affecting many areas of functioning. The core symptoms are deficits in social communication and social interaction, and restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviour, interests or activities. Many individuals with ASD also experience sensory difficulties, such as hypo- or hyper-sensitivity to light, sound, colour, smell, taste or touch.

The term 'spectrum' is used to indicate the wide range of abilities and difficulties associated with ASD. Intellectual ability can range from profound disability to superior intelligence; some individuals have no use of and/or minimal understanding of language while others have well developed vocabularies; symptom severity can also vary from very debilitating to relatively mild.

Diagnostic criteria

The most recent version of the US based Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-5; APA, 2013) specifies the following diagnostic criteria (see Table 1):

Table 1. ASD Diagnostic Criteria (DSM-5)

Deficits in Social Communication and Social Interaction

All of the following symptoms describing persistent deficits in social communication/interaction across contexts, not accounted for by general developmental delays, must be met:

1. Deficits in social-emotional reciprocity (e.g. abnormal social approach, failure of normal back-and-forth conversation; reduced sharing of interests, emotions, or affect; failure to initiate or respond to social interactions)

2. Deficits in nonverbal communicative behaviors used for social interaction (e.g., poorly integrated verbal and nonverbal communication; abnormalities in eye contact and body language; deficits in understanding and use of gestures; lack of facial expressions and nonverbal communication.)

3. Deficits in developing, maintaining, and understanding relationships (e.g.. difficulties adjusting behavior to suit various social contexts; difficulties in sharing imaginative play or in making friends; absence of interest in peers).

Restricted, Repetitive Patterns of Behavior, Interests or Activities

Two of the following four symptoms need to be present:

1. Stereotyped or repetitive motor movements, use of objects, or speech (e.g., simple motor stereotypies, lining up toys or flipping objects, echolalia, idiosyncratic phrases).

2. Insistence on sameness, inflexible adherence to routines, or ritualized patterns or verbal nonverbal behavior (e.g., extreme distress at small changes, difficulties with transitions, rigid thinking patterns, greeting rituals, need to take same route or eat food every day).

3.  Highly restricted, fixated interests that are abnormal in intensity or focus (e.g, strong attachment to or preoccupation with unusual objects, excessively circumscribed or perseverative interest).

4. Hyper- or hypo-reactivity to sensory input or unusual interests in sensory aspects of the environment (e.g., apparent indifference to pain/temperature, adverse response to specific sounds or textures, excessive smelling or touching of objects, visual fascination with lights or movement).

DSM-5 criteria state that symptoms must be present in early childhood but may not become fully manifest until social demands exceed capacities. Symptoms need to be functionally impairing and DSM-5 identifies three severity levels, depending on the amount of support required:: Level 1 (needing some support); 2 (needing substantial support); 3 (needing very substantial support). However, there is a lack of clarity about how these levels should be defined. DSM-5 also lists a number of specifiers that identify individual characteristics, i.e.: with/without accompanying intellectual impairment; with/without language impairment; associated with known medical or genetic condition or environmental exposure, or with another neurodevelopmental, mental or behavioral disorder.

The latest draft of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11; WHO 2018) generally follows DSM-5 in grouping symptoms into two core domains: (i) persistent deficits in the ability to initiate and sustain reciprocal social interaction and social communication; (ii) a range of restricted, repetitive, and inflexible patterns of behavior and interests. However, there is less emphasis on sensory sensitivities. To meet diagnostic criteria, symptoms must cause functional impairment although symptom severity may vary according to social or other contexts.  To reflect the wide range of ability in ASD ICD-11 also proposes that intellectual disability should be specifically rated as present/not present and functional language should be categorised as no/mild impairment; some impairment; no functional language. The validity and stability of these subdivisions are still to be established.

DSM-5 and ICD-11 diverge from their predecessors (DSM-IV and ICD-10) in that diagnosis is based on deficits in two core domains. Previously, diagnostic criteria specified deficits in three domains: communication; reciprocal social interaction, and restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of behaviour and interests). However, subsequent research demonstrated that deficits in communication and reciprocal social interaction almost invariably co-occurred and these two domains were combined. In a further departure from DSM-IV and ICD-10, ASD is no longer divided into sub-categories (i.e. autism, autistic disorder, atypical autism, Asperger syndrome/disorder; Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified {PDD-NOS}). Again, this decision was based on data demonstrating the lack of reliability of these separate diagnoses.  Concerns have been raised that individuals formerly diagnosed with these conditions could lose their autism diagnosis under the revised criteria and thus be denied necessary medical, social, educational or financial support. However, DSM-5 explicitly states: “Individuals with well-established DSM-IV diagnoses of autistic disorder, Asperger's disorder, or pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified, should be given the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder”.

Prevalence

Although ASD was once considered a very rare condition, prevalence estimates have risen steadily over past decades and recent reviews suggest rates of around 1% to 1.5% (Lyall et al., 2017), However, differences in sampling, screening, diagnostic instruments and ascertainment methods, mean that estimates vary widely from one study to another and the validity of much survey data remains in doubt (Fombonne, 2018). Nevertheless, it is clear that the numbers of individuals diagnosed with autism across the world have increased significantly over recent decades. Most evidence suggests that this growth reflects improvements in diagnostic practice and much greater awareness of ASD among the general public, and within clinical, social and educational services. Whether or not there has been an actual rise in prevalence has still to be demonstrated.

Early symptoms

The first signs of ASD are often evident by 18 to 24 months, with the earliest symptoms being associated with problems in social relating, reciprocal communication and/ or lack of imaginative play. Ritualistic and stereotyped behaviours may not become apparent until children are older - around 3-4 years. The preschool period (4-5 years), when children have to relate more closely with their peers, is often the time when autistic symptomatology becomes increasingly apparent. A quarter to one-third of parents report that, following an apparently normal period of development, their children lose speech and sometimes social skills around 18-24 months (Barger et al., 2013). The causes of such regression remain unknown but loss of skills tends to be more frequent in children of lower cognitive ability and in those with epilepsy or epileptoform activity.

Gender ratio

Like many conditions involving language impairments, the diagnosis of ASD is more frequent in males than females. The overall male:female ratio is estimated at around 3:1 (Loomes et al., 2017), although there is considerable variation between studies. Moreover, there are concerns that, because of subtle differences in the manifestation of ASD symptomatology in females, many girls and women who meet criteria nevertheless fail to receive a diagnosis. Until this diagnostic bias can be resolved the true male to female ratio will remain uncertain.

Diagnostic assessment

Although the typical age of diagnosis has decreased over recent decades many children do not receive a formal diagnosis until they are 4 or 5 years of age or even later. There is also great variation both within and between countries, with average age of first diagnosis ranging from 38 to 120 months (Brett et al., 2016; Daniels & Mandell, 2014). Family factors associated with delayed diagnosis include greater social deprivation and lower social/educational level. Diagnosis is also likely to be delayed in children with less severe autism symptoms or those who do not show a 'classic' picture of autism. This can particularly apply to girls, individuals with severe intellectual disabilities, or those who have additional sensory, physical or genetic impairments (deafness, blindness, Down's syndrome, cerebral palsy, etc).

There are no reliable bio-markers for ASD and hence diagnosis relies on the identification of a constellation of specific symptoms. Deficits in the core diagnostic domains must also be distinguished from deficits that may be attributable to factors such as low IQ, sensory impairments, or other developmental or psychiatric disorders. Valid diagnostic assessment relies on clinical observation, caregiver reports and a detailed developmental history. UK Nice Guidelines for both children and adults (2011; 2012) recommend that assessment should be conducted by a multidisciplinary team and that diagnosis is made on the basis of expert clinical judgment.

Diagnostic instruments

Although no single autism-specific instrument is recommended to diagnose ASD (NICE, 2011), a number of standardised instruments is available to enhance diagnostic accuracy. Table 2 lists commonly used screening instruments designed to identify individuals at risk of having ASD and who, if they approach suggested cut-off criteria, should be referred for a full diagnostic assessment (see also Hirota et al., 2018; McPheeters et al., 2016;).Table 3 lists measures that provide a structured framework for the detailed assessment of developmental and behavioural functioning and can be used as part of a more formal clinical assessment.  Interview-based information from carers may also be supplemented by standardised observational measures.

Table 2. Screening instruments for ASD.

Note: although individuals who meet cut-off scores on these instruments should be referred for further diagnostic assessment, failure to meet cut-off does not mean that ASD should be ruled out. If an individual achieves a low score, but other sources of information indicate developmental delay or disturbance, a full diagnostic assessment is still warranted.

Child assessments

Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-Chat, Robins et al., 1999). 23 item parent questionnaire

Screening Tool for Autism in Toddlers and Young Children (STAT, Stone, & Ousley, 2008). 20 minute observation for young children)

Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS; Schopler et al., 1986). 10-15 minute observational measure, supplemented by information from parents

Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ, Rutter et al., 2003). 10-15 minute questionnaire completed by main caregiver

Assessments that can be used with adults

Social Responsiveness Scales (SRS Constantino  & Gruber, 2012). 5-20 minute questionnaire with different versions for adults and children and self and informant report.

Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ, Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). 50-item self-report measure designed for adults; a 10-item version is also available as is an informant version for adolescents.

Ritvo Autism Asperger Diagnostic Scale – Revised (RAADS-R, Ritvo et al., 2011). 80 item self-report questionnaire

Table 3. Instruments recommended for aiding clinical diagnostic assessments

Observational:

Autism Diagnostic Observational Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 1999). Standardised observational measure focusing on reciprocal social communication skills. Different modules available for different ages and developmental levels. (45-60 minutes)

Interview measures:

Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R, Lord et al., 1994). Detailed interview focussing on early development and current functioning. (Around 1-2 hours). Often used in conjunction with ADOS (above)

Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders (DISCO, Leekham et al., 2002). Interview covering developmental history and current behaviour (Around 1-2 hours)

Developmental Dimensional and Diagnostic Interview (3di; Skuse et al., 2004)

Computer generated interview (90 minutes and 45-minute short form)

ADI-R, DISCO and 3Di require a carer or other informant who has known the individual from childhood. Each covers a wide range of behaviours (100+ items) but focus on communication and social deficits, and ritualistic and stereotyped behaviours. They require specialist training and can be used for diagnosis of both children and adults and across the IQ range (excluding profound intellectual disability)

Cognitive assessments

The diagnostic process should also ­incorporate assessments of cognitive and verbal ability. Psychometric tests suitable for use with young children with ASD include the Mullen Scales, the Merrill Palmer Scales, the Bayley Scales and the Wechsler Pre-School and Primary Scale of Intelligence; a wide choice of language tests is available (see Charman et al., 2017). Cognitive tests for older children and adults include the Wechsler scales (WISC, WASI & WAIS) and the Raven’s Matrices. The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Sparrow et al., 2005) can be helpful for assessing very young or severely intellectually impaired individuals, as they are based on carer information and can be used to assess functioning in several domains and across a wide age range. A combination of different methods - observational, carer-based and standardised assessments - is recommended in order to obtain a valid picture of an individual’s functioning across a range of different areas and contexts (Grondhuis et al., 2018).

Physical examination

Rates of physical problems in autism are high (Croen et al.,2015; Zerbo, 2018) and a comprehensive examination should be conducted to identify additional physical or neurological disorders. It is important, too, to assess vision, hearing and motor skills, as impairments in these areas may significantly affect general functioning. Routine neuroimaging is unlikely to be justified, unless there are unexplained changes in behaviour or loss of skills.NICE (2011) does not recommend routine genetic testing but suggests this should be done if there are specific dysmorphic features, congenital anomalies, other evidence of a learning or intellectual disability, or a family history of developmental disorders. However, whole genome sequencing is becoming increasingly more common across Europe and the US.

Epilepsy occurs in around 20-25% of individuals with ASD (Besag, 2018), but may not develop until the early teens, and it is important to be alert to the possibility of seizures if sudden, unexplained changes in behaviour occur. EEG abnormalities are also common but, in the absence of other symptoms, are not in themselves an indication for anti-epileptic medication.

Differential diagnosis

Research suggests that 70% or more of individuals with ASD meet criteria for one or more other disorders (APA, 2013; Mansour et al., 2017; Simonoff et al., 2008). These include intellectual disability, language disorders, specific learning disorders, ADHD, irritability and aggression, tics, and eating, sleeping and elimination disorders. Mental health problems, particularly anxiety and depression, are common in adolescence and adulthood. Routine screening, and treatment, for frequently co-occurring disorders should be part of any diagnostic assessment since these conditions can significantly reduce the quality of life of individuals with ASD and their families.

ASD is also more common in many genetic disorders than in the general population. These include Fragile X syndrome, Down syndrome, Rett syndrome, Cornelia de Lange syndrome, Tuberous Sclerosis Complex, Angelman Syndrome, Neurofibromatosis Type 1; Noonan syndrome; Williams syndrome, 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and many others (Richards et al., 2015). There is an increased risk of ASD among children who are profoundly deaf and those with congenital blindness (e.g. Do et al., 2017). However, families of children with an identified genetic or sensory disorder often find it very difficult to obtain a clinical assessment of ASD as many professionals tend to attribute all the child’s problems to the original diagnosis, even when development is atypical for that condition.

A number of other conditions affecting early development may be confused with ASD including developmental language disorders, severe psychosocial deprivation, profound intellectual impairment, selective mutism, early childhood schizophrenia, childhood disintegrative disorders and Landau-Klefner syndrome. However, the onset, course, response to intervention and family background in these disorders are generally different from those found in autism. In most cases, a detailed developmental and family history, together with careful physical, psychometric and linguistic assessments will avoid diagnostic confusion.

Causes


Genetics. Theories about the causes of autism have changed markedly over the years. When the condition was first described (Kanner, 1943) inadequate parenting was often considered to be the primary cause. However, it is now evident that families with one autistic child have a significantly increased risk of having other children with ASD. Family genetic studies have also identified very high concordance rates among monozygotic twins, and increased rates of social and communication difficulties and circumscribed interests or repetitive behaviours among relatives (Bolton et al.). In addition, there is an increased familial risk of depressive disorders. Currently ASD is recognised as a highly heritable genetic disorder that may be caused by multigene interactions or arise from spontaneous mutations in genes with major effects. A wide range of common and rare variants associated with ASD has been identified but these can differ from one individual to another. Many of the ASD-risk genes that have been identified are involved in chromatin remodeling, regulation of protein synthesis and degradation, or synaptic plasticity (Bourgeron, 2015).

Environment. Possible environmental factors linked with an increased risk of autism include higher parental age and a history of sub-optimal pre-or perinatal development (e.g. medication use in pregnancy; maternal obesity, hypertension or infection; Modabbernia et al., 2017). While there is no evidence that vaccination (notably the MMR vaccine) is associated with an increased risk of ASD, the potential impact of other environmental influences is still a focus of ongoing research.

Neurobiology. It is now generally accepted that autism is not due to impairment in a specific brain region or system but to widespread and diffuse effects on brain connectivity that affect perceptual, attentional, motor and social systems from the first year onwards. These effects become more evident when multisensory integration becomes important and it is then that key regions of the social brain may be particularly vulnerable to disruption (Elsabbagh & Johnson; 2016). Individual differences in sensitivity to the environment and different learning styles can subsequently result in very different developmental trajectories and may explain the very heterogeneous profiles of skills and deficits that characterise autism.

Cognitive and language ability

Estimated rates of intellectual disability in ASD vary widely and are influenced by the intelligence tests used and the samples of children involved. Data from a recent US survey (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2018) found that, among children with ASD, 44% had an IQ score in the average range (i.e. IQ ≥80), 24% scored in the borderline range (IQ 70-84) and 32% had intellectual disability (IQ score ≤70).

Verbal skills are generally more severely impaired than non-verbal skills but although the acquisition of language is typically delayed, around 70% of individuals with ASD develop some speech and over 40% - mainly those of higher IQ and with less severe autism symptoms - attain fluent speech (Wodka et al., 2013). However, language comprehension, particularly related to more complex or abstract language is typically poorer than expressive language.   This can give rise to difficulties since the relatively good spoken vocabulary of many individuals with ASD often gives a misleading impression of their true level of competence and understanding.

Interventions for autism

There is no evidence for treatments that claim to result in cures or recovery from autism. Moreover, many widely used interventions such as dietary and vitamin treatments, music therapy and sensory therapies have very limited evidence of effectiveness (NICE, 2013). NICE guidelines also discourage the use of a number of specific treatments (see Table 4).

Table 4. NICE (2013) recommendations for interventions that should be avoided

1. Do not use the following interventions to manage autism in any context in children and young people.

  • Secretin
  • Chelation
  • Hyperbaric oxygen therapy

2. Do not use neurofeedback or auditory integration training to manage speech and language problems in children and young people with autism.

3. Do not use omega‑3 fatty acids to manage sleep problems in children and young people with autism.

5. Do not use the following interventions for the management of core features of autism in children and young people

  • Antipsychotics
  • Antidepressants
  • Anticonvulsants
  • Exclusion   diets (e.g. gluten or casein free diets)

In recent years there has been growing evidence for the effectiveness of parent-mediated treatments that focus on the joint interaction between very young children and their caregivers. These include the Early Start Denver Model (ESDM), Joint Attention Symbolic Play Engagement and Regulation (JASPER) and Preschool Autism Communication Therapy (PACT) (see Lord et al., 2018). There is also moderate evidence for a number of programmes based on Applied Behaviour Analysis. These include Enhanced Milieu Teaching (EMT), Incidental teaching (IT), Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT), Reciprocal Imitation Training (RIT) and Social Communications/Emotional Regulation/Transactional Support (SCERTS) (Reichow et al., 2018). For older children, various social skills interventions have been found to improve social competence and understanding (Ke et al., 2018). Cognitive behavioural interventions to reduce anxiety also report moderate to high effect sizes (Lord et al., 2018).  High quality intervention research for adults is much more limited, although there is some evidence for the effectiveness of cognitive behaviour therapies (including mindfulness) for reducing anxiety and depression (Spain et al., 2017); supported employment schemes may also help to improve job finding and retention (Mavranezouli et al., 2013)

Medication

NICE guidelines (2013) highlight the fact that there are no medications that should be used for the management of core autism symptoms.  However, pharmacological interventions can be important in treating common co-occurring disorders such as irritability, agitation, ADHD, epilepsy, anxiety and mood disorders,

ASD in adulthood

Despite the fact that ASD is a life-long condition, much past research into diagnosis and treatment has focused on children. However, more recent studies have begun to highlight the needs and skills of adults.

Diagnosing ASD in adulthood

For individuals who remain undiagnosed during childhood but continue to show difficulties as they move through adolescence and into adulthood, NICE (2012) highlights a number of characteristics that should alert clinicians to the need for a full assessment of possible ASD (See Table 5).

Table 5. Symptoms in adulthood suggesting the need for a full autism diagnostic assessment

1. Adults of higher IQ

  • One or more of the following:
    • persistent   difficulties in social interaction
    • stereotypic   (rigid and repetitive) behaviours, resistance to change or restricted   interests and
  • One or   more of the following:
    • problems in obtaining or sustaining employment or   education
    • difficulties in initiating or sustaining social   relationships
    • previous or current contact with mental health or   learning disability services
    • a history   of a neurodevelopmental condition (including learning disabilities and   attention deficit hyperactivity disorder) or mental disorder

2. Adults with moderate or severe learning disability (using information from a family member or carer)

  • Two or   more of the following:
    • difficulties in reciprocal social interaction including:
  • limited interaction with others (e.g. being   aloof, indifferent or unusual)
  • interaction to fulfil needs only
  • interaction that is naïve or one-sided
    • lack of responsiveness to others
    • little or no change in behaviour in response to different social situations
    • limited social demonstration of empathy
    • rigid routines and resistance to change
    • marked repetitive activities (for example rocking or finger flapping) especially   when under stress or expressing emotion

Several of the diagnostic instruments initially designed for children (e.g. ADI-R) can be used reliably with adults. Although there may be problems in finding an appropriate informant if parents are deceased or very elderly, a satisfactory developmental history can frequently be obtained from siblings or other relatives. The ADOS also has a module that is specifically designed for adolescents and adults. Screening instruments include the AQ, RAADS-R and SRS (see Table 2).

Outcome in adulthood

Among the strongest predictors of a positive outcome are the development of language and having a non-verbal IQ in or around the average range (Lord et al., 2018). However, it is evident that outcome in adulthood, even for individuals of average IQ or above is often poor (see Howlin & Magiati, 2017).  Most people with ASD show improvements in many aspects of their development as they grow older and the severity of autism symptoms also tends to decrease over time. Nevertheless, access to higher education and employment is limited and follow-up studies suggest that fewer than a third of adults are in full-time employment. The majority remains dependent on families and/or state benefits for support and only a minority develops close, long-term relationships. There are few evidence-based interventions for adults and specialist help and support is particularly limited for individuals of average or above intelligence disability. Very many adults with ASD experience mental health problems that can greatly impair functioning and reduce quality of life and it is suggested that the constant demands of ‘fitting-in” to a non-autism world, and the absence of appropriately structured support or daily activities, all contribute to high levels of stress, anxiety and depression.

Physical problems and chronic health disorders are also more common than in the general population (Croen et al., 2015; Zerbo, 2018) and recent studies highlight the risk of premature mortality in ASD (Hirvikoski et al., 2016). Premature mortality is particularly high in adults with intellectual disability and epilepsy. In individuals of average or above average IQ suicide is a significant cause of premature death.

ASD in older adults

Very little is known about the lives of older individuals (i.e. 60 years+) and there is little information on whether, compared with the general population, these individuals show greater cognitive deterioration with age, or whether they show areas of relative preservation or strength. Although this theory is yet to be proven there is some evidence that, while verbal memory in ASD shows a similar decline as in “typical” ageing, visual and working memory skills tend to be better preserved. There may also be less deterioration in mental health and in overall quality of life in older adults with ASD than in the general elderly population (Roestorf & Bowler, 2016).

Conclusion

Understanding of ASD has improved significantly over recent decades.  It is now accepted that this condition is far more common than was once thought, that it can affect individuals of all cognitive levels, and that, in the majority of cases, it is genetically determined. However, more is still to be learned about the genetic mechanisms involved, and how these affect brain development and brain function throughout the life-span. Finally, thanks to a growing movement among individuals with ASD themselves, it is becoming increasingly recognised that ASD can be associated with many positive skills and potential benefits. Providing individuals with an inclusive and supportive environment that encompasses their “differences” and promotes well-being, rather than focusing only on deficits and difficulties, is a major challenge for the decade ahead.

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Publications.

Barger BD, Campbell JM and McDonough JD (2013) Prevalence and onset of regression within autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 43, 817–828.

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J. and Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders31(1), 5-17.

Besag, F.M. (2018). Epilepsy in patients with autism: links, risks and treatment challenges. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment14, 1-10.

Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Pickles, A., Rios, P.A., Goode, S., Crowson, M., Bailey, A. and Rutter, M., (1994). A case‐control family history study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry35(5), 877-900.

Bourgeron, T. (2015). From the genetic architecture to synaptic plasticity in autism spectrum disorder. Nature Reviews Neuroscience16(9), 551-563

Brett, D., Warnell, F., McConachie, H. and Parr, J.R. (2016). Factors affecting age at ASD diagnosis in UK: no evidence that diagnosis age has decreased between 2004 and 2014. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders46(6), 1974-1984.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC, 2016) Community report on autism. www.cdc.gov new autism data.

Charman, T., Hood, J. and Howlin, P. (2017). Psychological assessment in the clinical context. Rutter’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 5th Edition, pp.436-447. Chichester, Wiley.

Constantino, J.N. and Gruber, C.P. (2012). Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Croen, L.A., Zerbo, O., Qian, Y., Massolo, M.L., Rich, S., Sidney, S. and Kripke, C. (2015). The health status of adults on the autism spectrum. Autism19(7), 814-823.

Daniels, A.M. and Mandell, D.S. (2014). Explaining differences in age at autism spectrum disorder diagnosis: A critical review. Autism18(5), 583-597.

Do, B., Lynch, P., Macris, E. M., Smyth, B., Stavrinakis, S., Quinn, S., and Constable, P. A. (2017). Systematic review and meta‐analysis of the association of Autism Spectrum Disorder in visually or hearing impaired children. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics37(2), 212-224

Elsabbagh, M. and Johnson, M.H. (2016). Autism and the social brain: The first-year puzzle. Biological Psychiatry80(2), 94-99.

Fombonne, E. (2018). The rising prevalence of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry59(7), 717-720.

Grondhuis, S.N., Lecavalier, L., Arnold, L.E., Handen, B.L., Scahill, L., McDougle, C.J. and Aman, M.G. (2018). Differences in verbal and nonverbal IQ test scores in children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders49, 47-55.

Hirota, T., So, R., Kim, Y.S., Leventhal, B. and Epstein, R.A., (2018). A systematic review of screening tools in non-young children and adults for autism spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities80,.1-12.

Hirvikoski, T., Mittendorfer-Rutz, E., Boman, M., Larsson, H., Lichtenstein, P. and Bölte, S. (2016). Premature mortality in autism spectrum disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry208(3),.232-238.

Howlin, P. and Magiati, I. (2017). Autism spectrum disorder: Outcomes in adulthood. Current Opinion in Psychiatry30(2), 69-76.

Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child2(3), 217-250.

Ke, F., Whalon, K. and Yun, J., (2018). Social skill interventions for youth and adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder: a systematic review. Review of Educational Research88(1), pp.3-42.

Le Couteur, A. Lord, C., and Rutter, M. (2003). The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Leekam, S.R., Libby, S.J., Wing, L., Gould, J. and Taylor, C. (2002). The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders: algorithms for ICD‐10 childhood autism and Wing and Gould autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry43(3), 327-342.

Loomes, R., Hull, L. and Mandy, W.P.L. (2017). What is the male-to-female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry56(6), 466-474.

Lord, C., Elsabbagh, M., Baird, G. and Veenstra-Vanderweele, J., 2018. Autism spectrum disorder. The Lancet, 392,11–17 August 2018, 508-520

Lord C., Rutter M., DiLavore P. and Risi S. (1999) Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Lyall, K., Croen, L., Daniels, J., Fallin, M. D., Ladd-Acosta, C., Lee, B. K., ... and Windham, G. C. (2017). The changing epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. Annual Review of Public Health38, 81-102.

McPheeters, M. L., Weitlauf, A., Vehorn, A., Taylor, C., Sathe, N. A., Krishnaswami, S., ... and Warren, Z. E. (2016). Screening for Autism Spectrum Disorder in Young Children. A Systematic Evidence Review for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. Rockville (MD): Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (US); 2016 Feb.

Mansour, R., Dovi, A. T., Lane, D. M., Loveland, K. A., and Pearson, D. A. (2017). ADHD severity as it relates to comorbid psychiatric symptomatology in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Research in Developmental Disabilities60, 52-64.

Mavranezouli, I., Megnin-Viggar O., Cheema., Howlin, P., Baron-Cohen S. and Pilling, S. (2013)  The cost-effectiveness of supported employment for adults with autism in the UK.  Autism, 18(8) 975-984.

Modabbernia, A. Velthorst, E. and Reichenberg, A. (2017). Environmental risk factors for autism: an evidence-based review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Molecular Autism8(1), 13. DOI 10.1186/s13229-017-0121-4

NICE (National Institute for Health Care and Excellence). (2011; updated 2017) Autism spectrum disorder in under 19s: recognition, referral and diagnosis. Clinical guideline [CG128] Published date: September 2011, updated: December 2017

NICE (2012, updated 2016) Autism spectrum disorder in adults: diagnosis and management Clinical guideline [CG142] Published date: June 2012, updated: August 2016

NICE (2013) Autism spectrum disorder in under 19s: support and management Clinical guideline [CG170] Published date: August 2013

Reichow, B., Hume, K., Barton, E.E. and Boyd, B.A. (2018). Early intensive behavioral intervention (EIBI) for young children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, (5).

Richards, C., Jones, C., Groves, L., Moss, J., and Oliver, C. (2015). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder phenomenology in genetic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry2(10), 909-916.

Ritvo, E.R., Guthrie, D., Ritvo, M.J., Hufnagel, D.H., McMahon, W., Tonge, B., Mataix-Cols, D., Jassi, A., Attwood, T. and Eloff, J. (2011). The Ritvo Autism Asperger Diagnostic Scale-Revised (RAADS-R): a scale to assist the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in adults: an international validation study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders41(8), 1076-1089

Robins, D.L., Fein, D., Barton, M. and Green, J.A., 1999. The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT). Self-published.

Roestorf, A. and Bowler, D. (2016) Ageing and psychological functioning in autism spectrum disorder. In SD Wright (Ed) Autism Spectrum Disorder in Mid and Later Life. p.222-232. London, Jessica Kingsley Publications.

Rutter, M., Bailey, A. and Lord, C. (2003). SCQ. The Social Communication Questionnaire. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A. and Lord, C., 2003. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services,

Schopler E, Reichler R J, Renner B R.  (1986) The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) for Diagnostic Screening and Classification of Autism. New York: Irvington.

Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T. and Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry47(8), 921-929.

Skuse, D., Warrington, R., Bishop, D., Chowdhury, U., Lau, J., Mandy, W. and Place, M. (2004). The developmental, dimensional and diagnostic interview (3di): A novel computerized assessment for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry43(5), pp.548-558.

Spain, D., Sin, J., Chalder, T., Murphy, D. and Happe, F. (2015). Cognitive behaviour therapy for adults with autism spectrum disorders and psychiatric co-morbidity: A review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders9,151-162.

Sparrow, S.S., Cicchetti, D.V. and Balla, D.A. (2005). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales:(Vineland II), survey interview form/caregiver rating form. Livonia, MN: Pearson Assessments.

Stone, W. and Ousley, O.Y. (2008). Screening Tool for Autism in Toddlers and Young Children (STAT). Vanderbilt University.

Wodka, E. L., Mathy, P. and Kalb, L. (2013). Predictors of phrase and fluent speech in children with autism and severe language delay. Pediatrics, peds-2012. oi:10.1542/peds.2012-2221

WHO (2018) The 11th Revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11)

Zerbo, O., Qian, Y., Ray, T., Sidney, S., Rich, S., Massolo, M. and Croen, L.A. (2018). Healthcare service utilization and cost among adults with autism spectrum disorders in a US Integrated Healthcare System. Autism in Adulthood, on line, doi.org/10.1089/aut.2018.0004

This article was published on this site in October 2018. It replaces the article previously published on this site in 2002.